About science About vision

[00:05 - 00:09]
This is about science produced by the California Institute of Technology
[00:09 - 00:14]
and originally broadcast by station KPCC Pasadena California. The
[00:14 - 00:19]
programs are made available to the station by national educational radio.
[00:19 - 00:24]
This program is about vision with host Dr. Peter less a man and his
[00:24 - 00:26]
guest Dr. Derrick Fandor.
[00:26 - 00:30]
Here now is Dr. listen.
[00:30 - 00:35]
Have you ever taken the time to think about that old phrase the mind's eye.
[00:35 - 00:40]
Of course what it means is the ability to remember something in picture form to see
[00:40 - 00:45]
something as ones who saw it. But that phrase comes close to telling us how things
[00:45 - 00:50]
really are. The eye without the brain's ability to translate what it sees
[00:50 - 00:55]
is useless while the mind without the eyes ability to recognize and tell
[00:55 - 00:59]
about light is limited. So we see that this business of seeing is a
[00:59 - 01:04]
cooperative venture that works so well for most of us that we seldom realize what's
[01:04 - 01:09]
going on. Scientists have known for years of course that the eye is a device for
[01:09 - 01:14]
detecting light changing the light it receives into coded nerve impulses
[01:14 - 01:19]
and sending them along the optic nerve to the brain. But what are these impulses.
[01:19 - 01:24]
Are they tiny pictures long messages or maybe bits and pieces of
[01:24 - 01:29]
numbers similar to what would work in a computer. That is what some of the latest work
[01:29 - 01:33]
is being aimed about specifically at how the brain gets what it
[01:33 - 01:38]
gets from the eye what form the information is in where it goes in
[01:38 - 01:43]
the brain and what the brain does with the information across CVS.
[01:43 - 01:47]
Here to discuss these problems with us now is Dr. Derek Fender professor of
[01:47 - 01:52]
biology and Applied Science accounting. Dr. fender and his colleagues have been
[01:52 - 01:57]
working with a very small electric signals generated by the optic centers of the brain
[01:57 - 02:02]
when the eyes see flashing lights. Dr. fender a native of Heath
[02:02 - 02:07]
England obtained his bachelor's degree at Reading University in 1939 and his
[02:07 - 02:11]
Ph.D. in physics at the same university in 1956.
[02:11 - 02:15]
He was a senior lecturer at the university before joining the Kel-Tec faculty in
[02:15 - 02:20]
1961. Derek friend has done research on the function of eye
[02:20 - 02:25]
movements in the visual process and is now engaged in biological systems
[02:25 - 02:30]
analysis. Derek What exactly does and how does the
[02:30 - 02:34]
brain analyze visual information and recognize what the eyes see.
[02:34 - 02:42]
Well before before I get into that problem itself I would like to
[02:42 - 02:47]
give due credit to a colleague of mine who works with me in this field.
[02:47 - 02:51]
This is Dietrich Lehmann who is a neuro
[02:51 - 02:56]
psychiatrist who actually works at the Institute for visual Sciences in
[02:56 - 03:01]
San Francisco. We run this as a cooperative research project.
[03:01 - 03:06]
So you are really speaking just as much for himself or for here as for yours you must blame
[03:06 - 03:11]
him for at least half of what I thought you were being very noble in sharing the credit
[03:11 - 03:15]
for to have already taken the way out.
[03:15 - 03:21]
As for the problem itself this is really a very big problem.
[03:21 - 03:25]
Just how the human brain analyzes the visual information and
[03:25 - 03:31]
enables us to recognize specific things in the visual field. There are
[03:31 - 03:36]
many people in fact working many research teams working on this all over America
[03:36 - 03:41]
and in fact all over the rest of the world and they're attacking this problem at
[03:41 - 03:46]
various levels and you'll see what I mean by levels in a few moments I
[03:46 - 03:51]
hope we ourselves are interested in one
[03:51 - 03:56]
very small level of this information chain and that is what
[03:56 - 04:01]
actually happens. Do we believe visual information as it
[04:01 - 04:06]
makes its way from the eye to the visual cortex of the
[04:06 - 04:12]
brain. Now that in fact is a long path.
[04:12 - 04:16]
If if I can just trace this the
[04:16 - 04:22]
passage of the information over this path with you it goes something like this. Jerry could I
[04:22 - 04:26]
interrupt for a second to ask as a very simple person what is the cortex
[04:26 - 04:31]
to which this message is transmitted and the cortex is specifically
[04:31 - 04:36]
the surface layers of the brain or the surface layers
[04:36 - 04:41]
of nerves. And we usually refer to reserve the term
[04:41 - 04:45]
cortex for these layers of nerve cells which
[04:45 - 04:51]
respond to primary sensory input. That is if you like goes into
[04:51 - 04:56]
some visual cortex like an active sound in the
[04:56 - 05:01]
ear then some cells in the auditory cortex become active and so on.
[05:01 - 05:06]
But these are all surface layer cells not the deep seeded cells
[05:06 - 05:11]
which perform more fundamental processes of the
[05:11 - 05:16]
thinking and knowing and making
[05:16 - 05:20]
abstractions proving theorems. And so on.
[05:20 - 05:25]
So this is this is sort of the entry point it's like the mailbox of
[05:25 - 05:30]
the brain that's where the messages come in first the cortex is really the first
[05:30 - 05:35]
point at which the brain receives information from the outside world.
[05:35 - 05:39]
Yes this is quite true. Now if I can get back to my
[05:39 - 05:45]
to my long or long route the like from the
[05:45 - 05:50]
outside world goes in through the eye through the pupil
[05:50 - 05:55]
and through the lens of the eye in his image on the retina or the simple textbooks
[05:55 - 06:00]
have pictures of cameras at this point and compare the eye with a camera. And in fact it
[06:00 - 06:05]
behaves much the same as a camera up to that point. That is the image is upside down and
[06:05 - 06:08]
backwards and so on in the psyche.
[06:08 - 06:15]
The retina images formed on the retina of the retina is at the
[06:15 - 06:20]
back of the eye inside the eyeball. And it is a mosaic of
[06:20 - 06:25]
retinal receptors. There are the hundred million or so of these in each
[06:25 - 06:29]
eye. They have visual pigments in them. The pigment
[06:29 - 06:34]
absorbs the light and in the act of absorbing the light it
[06:34 - 06:39]
changes the light signal into an electrical signal.
[06:39 - 06:44]
A very small electrical signal which flows off as a
[06:44 - 06:49]
nerve impulse and this is the first point at which a change is being made.
[06:49 - 06:54]
But from there on the light and the picture the visual scene has been lost.
[06:54 - 06:59]
The brain only has nerve impulses little pulses of electricity on which to
[06:59 - 07:01]
do its work.
[07:01 - 07:07]
The amount of information encoded by the retina such as a small only
[07:07 - 07:12]
twelve brightness and color probably the code brightness and color
[07:12 - 07:16]
into nerve impulses and usually the coding is made by the
[07:16 - 07:20]
rapidity at which these pulses follow one after the other.
[07:20 - 07:28]
The nerve impulses then flow off from the retina to the brain
[07:28 - 07:33]
through a big bundle of nerves about a million there was in the bundle called the
[07:33 - 07:38]
optic nerve. This nerve makes its way
[07:38 - 07:42]
from the two are used with nerves make their way in place from the two
[07:42 - 07:47]
ears through the quiet ASM which is just behind the eyes underneath the brain which
[07:47 - 07:51]
some of the nerves cross over but some of the nerves do not
[07:51 - 07:58]
on their way to the brain. You remember of course of the brain is split into
[07:58 - 08:02]
two hemispheres left and right hemisphere. The crossing of the
[08:02 - 08:07]
nerves in the Kiowas is such that if you were to look
[08:07 - 08:12]
exactly straighter and or move the visual information which is out to your left
[08:12 - 08:16]
hand side finishes in the right hand hemisphere
[08:16 - 08:22]
of the brain and all that which is up to the right hand side finishes in the left hemisphere of
[08:22 - 08:26]
the brain. This is of course quite typical of a
[08:26 - 08:31]
human but the control the sensory input is
[08:31 - 08:36]
crossed and the control from the brain to the limbs is crossed. Left side of
[08:36 - 08:38]
the brain controls the right hand and so on.
[08:38 - 08:40]
Do we have any idea why that is so.
[08:40 - 08:47]
Why you have a crossing is necessary. You know that's a very difficult problem to answer.
[08:47 - 08:53]
If you follow up through the development of the various animals you
[08:53 - 08:58]
find that that the insect level there is no crossing left
[08:58 - 09:02]
his left and right is right. But they don't get mixed up as you go up to
[09:02 - 09:07]
higher levels. Certainly by the time you get to things
[09:07 - 09:12]
like frogs then there is a crossing of some information but
[09:12 - 09:17]
other information is not crossed. And finally by the time you get to things as advanced as cats
[09:17 - 09:22]
say then this crossing has occurred. But at what stage in the life or in the
[09:22 - 09:27]
evolution of the animal kingdom. The decision was made to
[09:27 - 09:32]
cross the hemispheres completely like this is really very difficult to say and the reason for which
[09:32 - 09:37]
the crossing occurred so anyhow the information passed through the chi or somewhere it's
[09:37 - 09:40]
crossed and.
[09:40 - 09:47]
Essentially left goes in to right and right goes to the left.
[09:47 - 09:51]
And this information finishes in the cortex. We've been talking about
[09:51 - 09:56]
before but specifically in the visual cortex and the
[09:56 - 10:01]
cortex itself is quite large. It is as many subdivisions. They all
[10:01 - 10:06]
have silly Latin names but also fortunately have numbers and we are
[10:06 - 10:11]
talking specifically today about areas 17 18 and 19 of the visual
[10:11 - 10:16]
cortex. I was 17 18 and 19 are a lot of the back of your head if you know so
[10:16 - 10:20]
interlaced your fingers and put your hands behind your head in a comfortable relaxed
[10:20 - 10:25]
gesture the palms of your hands would be neatly fitting over
[10:25 - 10:30]
the area 17 in each hemisphere of your heart of your brain.
[10:30 - 10:38]
OK well now that is the first relay station in the brain as it were. These are the primary
[10:38 - 10:42]
projection in the areas where sensory stimuli
[10:42 - 10:47]
first become available to the brain itself and the point at which the brain can
[10:47 - 10:52]
first start working on the little electrical impulses which
[10:52 - 10:57]
are representing the visual scene from the outside
[10:57 - 11:02]
world fact that the brain still has to make something of this message. Yeah that is
[11:02 - 11:06]
true the perception has to go on perception that is
[11:06 - 11:11]
converting it into a picture which we see in the mind's eye you that you talked about earlier on
[11:11 - 11:17]
perception goes on in other areas of the brain which I could just call the higher centers the
[11:17 - 11:22]
higher levels of the brain. And my work specifically
[11:22 - 11:26]
today stops at areas 17 18 19. And
[11:26 - 11:33]
perception in higher levels is another problem not only another problem
[11:33 - 11:37]
for me but another problem for other people who are not wonder what about the coding
[11:37 - 11:41]
mechanism that that is used.
[11:41 - 11:41]
Well.
[11:41 - 11:47]
This of course brings us really to the next big problem. Just what sort of information
[11:47 - 11:52]
is transmitted from the outside world to
[11:52 - 11:57]
the cortex. I have said that the retinal intercept is really only sample
[11:57 - 12:02]
brightness and color in the visual world
[12:02 - 12:07]
and it would be possible just to transmit this information and
[12:07 - 12:12]
no more to the cortex and then to use the rest of the brain as a
[12:12 - 12:16]
big computer like make an ism to figure it all out and figure it all out. Yes
[12:16 - 12:22]
there are other ways of doing it however and the
[12:22 - 12:28]
one of the other way is to start. Making
[12:28 - 12:33]
abstractions of the visual field as far forward in the visual
[12:33 - 12:37]
system as you can I mean as close to the to the eyeball as you
[12:37 - 12:42]
can by abstractions I mean things
[12:42 - 12:47]
like straight lines edges movement. It would be
[12:47 - 12:52]
perfectly possible to detect the use parameters of the
[12:52 - 12:57]
scene. And rather than transmit all of this hash about brightness
[12:57 - 13:02]
to transmit the information which say it is there is a straight line
[13:02 - 13:07]
in this part of the visual field. This is a
[13:07 - 13:12]
bank tree produces a reduction of information and many
[13:12 - 13:17]
conserved. Capacity in the nerve
[13:17 - 13:22]
fibers and in the brain when the brain has to perform its final
[13:22 - 13:23]
analysis of the picture.
[13:23 - 13:28]
So in the characteristically scientific way you strive to use the
[13:28 - 13:32]
simplest of model in the simplest of our impasse to find out how the
[13:32 - 13:34]
system works.
[13:34 - 13:39]
Yes that is true although it's very difficult to
[13:39 - 13:44]
say what is simplest in this case. You see this coding principle
[13:44 - 13:49]
has been examined on animals and animals you
[13:49 - 13:53]
actually open up the brain of the animal you stick an electrode into a
[13:53 - 13:58]
cell in area 17 in the
[13:58 - 14:02]
cortex and then you run around in front of the animal and wave a straight line in
[14:02 - 14:07]
his visual field until eventually if you have chosen the right
[14:07 - 14:12]
cell in the cortex you find that that cell fires
[14:12 - 14:18]
when the straight line is in the right position. No straight lines seem
[14:18 - 14:23]
to be pretty simple but are really pretty simple to one animal. In fact the work which has
This program has been transcribed using automated software tools, made possible through a collaboration between the American Archive of Public Broadcasting and Pop Up Archive. Please note that no automated transcription is perfect nor is it intended to replace human transcription labor. If you would like to contribute corrections to this transcript, please contact MITH at mith@umd.edu.